Sunday, November 12, 2006

Cuckolder kids stick together

Why do animals help each other? At first glance, helping behavior seems to stand in stark contrast to the notion that evolution is driven by the 'survival of the fittest'. This paradox can be resolved by considering relatedness: individuals may enhance the transmission of their own genes by helping individuals who possess many of the same genes (termed 'inclusive fitness'). Celebrated examples of individuals helping others, with no apparent benefit to themselves, include: (1) birds that help feed young at relatives' nests, and (2) ground squirrels that warn of impending danger when relatives are nearby. In both, inclusive fitness has been invoked to explain the evolution of this otherwise perplexing behavior.

From this begs is the question: How do animals know who to help? For sexually monogamous species, the answer comes easily; they should help nestmates. In sexually promiscuous species, however, individuals cannot reliably assume nestmates are siblings, and so require a more direct mechanism for differentiating kin from non-kin. This could be accomplished by evaluating the phenotype of potential kin relative to one's own phenotype, a technique known as self-referent kin recognition.

Bluegill sunfish are an ideal model organism with which to test for an association between promiscuity and the evolution of self-referent kin recognition. Males are either parentals or cuckolders (left and right, respectively, in above photo); the former tend nests and secure most of the paternity, while the latter forgo parental duties and instead sneak sperm into parental nests during spawning. Bluegills may gain inclusive fitness benefits by cooperating with shoal-mates that are kin. During such interactions, parental offspring can reliably assume a certain degree of relatedness with most individuals that originated from the same nest. Cuckolder offspring cannot, so they would benefit more from actively discriminating kin from non-kin using phenotypic cues.

Tim Hain - a graduate student from the University of Western Ontario, renowned for his prowess on the dance floor - and his supervisor, Bryan Neff, embarked on a series of elegant experiments to test kin recognition mechanisms in Bluegills from Lake Opinicon. Larvae of known parentage were presented with a choice between two odors originating from broods of varying degrees of relatedness. As predicted, cuckolder offspring were more likely to associate with the odors of relatives, while parental offspring showed no such association. This study was therefore able to show that, even within a species, promiscuity can be a driving force behind the evolution of self-referent kin recognition.

If you find yourself intrigued and eager to learn more, don't go looking for Tim at Western. He is currently doing fieldwork on guppies/establishing himself on dance floors in Trinidad. Though if you pay QUBS a visit next summer, keep an eye out for Tim’s distinctive 'dominance markings' (see photo) and you can’t go wrong.

Hain TJA, Neff BD (2006) Promiscuity drives self-referent kin recognition. Current Biology 16: 1807-1811 [Full Text]

6 Comments:

At 11/14/2006 3:22 PM, Anonymous Anonymous said...

Nice Paper Tim, well done!

I am however challenging you to a dominance battle in the form of a dance off.

Please prepare yourself accordingly and limber up.

C-Note

 
At 11/14/2006 4:10 PM, Anonymous Anonymous said...

C-Note, you made your bed, now you have to lie in it. If you knew any better, you'd just stay lyin' 'cus I be bringing my bag of tricks, and that bag is full, brutha.

Timbo the Himbo

 
At 11/14/2006 9:30 PM, Blogger Katie Langin said...

Why not a rock-paper-scissors battle? It didn't work out so well for Tim last time around...

 
At 11/29/2006 2:15 PM, Anonymous Anonymous said...

Stay tuned for an academic brawlfest with Robert Hammond and Laurent Keller.

I am readying my dominance markings.

 
At 5/08/2007 3:56 AM, Anonymous Anonymous said...

i need some publication evidence for this so-called kin-reference "crap', cause i really don't buy any of it yet! and i studied and directly filmed and monitored mating of BG and PS in Opinicon for years. i know fish smell but do YOU really believe they can smell kin? maybe it's me that's missing something relevant (research wise), for the past years i've been out of this avenue of cuckoldry behaviour. lemme know what you think (first ask mart and dave if i have relevant experience to ask these questions.

blake konkle

 
At 5/08/2007 5:52 PM, Blogger Katie Langin said...

Hi Blake,
Thanks for your interest in the post. I suggest you go to the "Full Text" link and read the paper for yourself, then decide whether or not you believe bluegills can "smell" their kin. Feel free to post specific questions/comments here regarding the paper.

Katie

 

Post a Comment

<< Home